Sign in Register Submit Manuscript

Hapres Home

Location: Home >> Detail

J Psychiatry Brain Sci. 2024;9:e240006. https://doi.org/10.20900/jpbs.20240006

Review

Training of Awareness in ADHD: Leveraging Metacognition

Agatha Lenartowicz 1,* , Brett DeSchepper 2 , Gregory V. Simpson 2

1 Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, OneMind Staglin Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA

2 Think Now Inc., San Francisco, CA 94105, USA

* Correspondence: Agatha Lenartowicz.

Received: 04 September 2024; Accepted: 30 September 2024; Published: 09 October 2024

ABSTRACT

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a disorder that is prevalent in children and adults, with significant impact on life outcomes. Common treatment strategies include a combination of pharmacological and psychosocial interventions which have recognized limits to their effectiveness. Consequently, there exists interest in additional non-pharmacological interventions. In the current minireview we aim to complement existing surveys by focusing on a complementary approach, namely rooted in metacognition or the training of awareness. We review programs that incorporate metacognitive training of awareness in skill-training, psychosocial interventions, and mindfulness, and discuss existing assessments of metacognitive ability in ADHD. Existing data suggest that metacognitive approaches have potential in supporting symptom management in ADHD, with gains in objective assessments in near and far transfer tasks in educational research and high satisfaction from parents. Further research is warranted in assessment of the relative contribution of metacognitive elements relative to other treatment components, objective assessments of outcomes in psychosocial interventions, and efficacy in adult interventions.

KEYWORDS: ADHD; awareness; metacognition; psychosocial; training; non-pharmaceutical

INTRODUCTION

Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized by symptoms of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity [1] that have significant consequences on multiple aspects of life including social, educational, and professional [2–5]. Its prevalence rates are around 3% [2,6] for adults and 5%–7% for children [2,7], with 60% persistence into adulthood [8]. Current treatments include psychosocial and pharmacological interventions [9,10]. Medications (including stimulants and non-stimulants) have a high-reported efficacy in reducing symptoms [11,12]. However, they are accompanied by mixed concerns about side-effects [13–15], limitations in cases of substance abuse co-morbidity [16], and issues of adherence [17–19]. Thus, non-pharmacological alternative or complementary interventions continue to be of significant interest as evidenced by numerous reviews [20–24], with greatest empirical evidence amassed in support of psychosocial interventions [20,23,25], and increasingly also neurofeedback [26,27] and physical exercise [23,24,28,29], with data also emerging to inform efficacy of digital [22] and mindfulness-based [30] interventions.

In the current minireview we aim to complement these existing surveys by focusing on another key element that has emerged in non-pharmacological interventions, namely metacognition or the training of awareness. Metacognition refers to knowledge of one’s cognition (i.e., self-knowledge or self-awareness of one’s cognitive and learning processes), as well as related metacognitive skills and experiences [31]. It includes regulation of cognition, encapsulating planning, monitoring and evaluating behavior, and translating into predicting, checking and adjusting of one’s cognitive processes [32]. Critically, such self-regulation has a demonstrated role in improving learning outcomes in the educational domain [33–37], with a proposed role in supporting far-transfer [38], and a similarly recognized role in therapeutic action in psychiatry [39,40], potentially by supporting the self-awareness of and action on maladaptive neurobehavioral processes rather than content [41]. These elements of metacognitive training may render it particularly appropriate to addressing a key challenge in ADHD, the recognized heterogeneity in etiology, symptoms, cognitive deficits, and medication responses in this population [2,42–48], by targeting self-regulation on an individual basis [21]. The goal of this minireview is to present a current survey of published interventions in ADHD that feature metacognitive training.

Metacognitive Training in ADHD

To identify published works that include metacognition in ADHD interventions, we conducted three searches using Web-of-Science (Clarivate), PubMed and Google Scholar literature databases. The search was conducted with key words “metacognition”, “awareness” and “ADHD” included in the topic or abstract field. We contacted original authors for works not available through library resources, and identified additional articles based on current reviews. We also conducted a future search (i.e., articles that cited found articles) for a more complete search. We eliminated works that reported on “awareness of ADHD” in educators, medical practitioners or within broader society (i.e., were not empirical intervention studies). This search produced a total of 17 empirical works (Tables 1 & 2) on the role of metacognitive training in treatments for ADHD that could be classified into categories based on the complementary treatment goal. These include (1) skill training (e.g., executive function or working memory) most prevalent in efforts targeting educational outcomes; (2) psychosocial interventions (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy) dominant in clinical settings and which include parental training; and (3) existing mindfulness interventions adapted to ADHD. We review each category in turn, with additional consideration of key factors such as metacognitive ability.

Metacognition in skill training

Educational research paradigms

A systematic effort to implement metacognitive awareness training into ADHD treatment has been documented within educational research, where the role of metacognition in self-regulated learning has demonstrated value for improving educational outcomes [35,36], especially in promoting far transfer [34,38]. One of the first such training programs was developed in 2013 by Garcia-Madruga et al. [49] to target reading comprehension in a small sample of 8–9-year-olds (15 in experimental condition and 16 in control). Motivated by a need to improve far-transfer effects, they included a broad range of activities, natural learning contexts and progressive increases in difficulty, along with iterative refinements of training materials. They reported gains in both reading comprehension, as well as visuo spatial and working memory assessments relative to a control (d = 0.25–0.79). Subsequent studies [50–52] elaborated on the proposed 60 min training sessions to bookend a series of activities targeting a specific skill, with a metacognitive introduction at the start and metacognitive reflection at the end. Such training sessions would then be delivered to groups of students in a classroom setting, for a total of 1–2 sessions/week, over 8 to 11 weeks. Statistically significant gains from metacognitive training across these studies were demonstrated for training of problem solving [52] as well as reading & listening comprehension [50,51]. Notably the gains were replicated in larger sample sizes (e.g., n = 64–69/training group [51,52]), in a cross-over design [52] (with all groups receiving treatment at some point in the study) and relative to both active (non-metacognitive training program) and passive (standard school experience) control groups [50,51].

This effort was followed by three studies [53–55] where a modification of the training program was developed to target working memory and executive function abilities in kids with ADHD (Age 5.2–8.5 years, Table 1). Across the studies the same metacognitive training session structure (i.e., metacognitive strategy and reflection with feedback book-ending a topical training activity) was applied to support training of working memory [53–55], as well as inhibition [55]. In these studies, groups of kids with and without ADHD were assigned into both training and control groups (13–20 individuals per group). In the control group kids participated in typical school activity in lieu of intervening training but had identical pre and post study assessment. Moderate significant gains were observed in objective assessments of working memory [53] and inhibition [55] in the training group for kids with ADHD, but not in the non-training group. Additionally, training gains were not significant for most measures for typically developing peers. Notably, assessment of transfer effects [54] showed that following metacognitive training of working memory, gains were also observed in performance on inhibition and attention tasks, and nonverbal reasoning, with gains significant across several measures in a 1 month follow up. Finally, a study by Pisacco et al. [56] in older children (Age = 13.1 years, n = 47), employing two different metacognitive programs of similar dosage structure, showed gains in written expression that persisted in a 3-month follow up. Interestingly, across these four studies, gains were inconsistent in parent/teacher self-report measures of ADHD symptoms, which may suggest that the extent of transfer is limited to cognitive performance or that the self-report symptoms capture a different dimension of behavior than the performance assessments.

TABLE 1
Table 1. Metacognitive & skill training in ADHD.

Role of skills versus metacognition

An important limitation of the reviewed metacognitive training is that, in application to ADHD, the studies did not separate the relative contributions of skill-specific training (e.g., working memory activities) versus the metacognitive elements to the gains in outcomes. However, some evidence exists to suggest that the metacognitive elements may have a key role. An earlier study using the same session structure for training of reading comprehension in typically developing children (9–11 years), did include an active control group (n = 57) who received the same reading training program as the experimental group (n = 45), but without the metacognitive elements [51]. They reported gains over the active control group with moderate to large effects sizes (d = 0.6–1.2) and sustained gains to reading in an 8 month follow up. Similarly, Zheng et al. [57] added about 10 minutes of metacognitive reflection at the end of an astronomy science class in 5th grade students with ADHD, and showed that after 15 classes and in a 2-week follow-up, gains in scientific knowledge (math, spatial) and learning motivation had greater learning-effect sizes in kids with the added reflection (n = 49) than those without (n = 48). Finally, Kajka & Kulik in a similar age group (Age = 10.4 years), showed that training only on metacognitive thinking via the so-called mind maps learning strategy which uses visual notes to organize idea development, after 25 training sessions using a variety of idea topics, significantly increased verbal fluency in kids with ADHD [58]. Thus, some evidence exists to support learning gains in kids with ADHD when metacognitive reflection is systematically incorporated into skill training.

In contrast, several studies have implemented ecological training programs of executive function skills, but with emphasis on skill acquisition more than metacognitive reflection. Such studies report gains in self-reported symptoms, but lack gains on objective assessments [61], consistency in gains across assessments [59,60] or persistence of gains in follow up [59]. Namely, Paananen et al. [59] compared an established interactive executive function coaching system (Maltti) in school age children with ADHD (Age = 9.5 years, n = 77) in Finland, and after a weekly session for the duration of a school-semester, reported improvements in cognitive control (i.e., inattentive) symptoms but not hyperactive symptoms, and the follow-up gains did not differ from those in a waitlist control group (n = 77). Tamm et al. [60] examined a commercial program (Pay Attention! [62]), designed to train sustained, selective, alternating, and divided attention via flexible coaching sessions. Following 16 bi-weekly sessions (Age = 9.1 years, n = 54), as compared to a waitlist group (Age = 9.5 years, n = 51) significant improvement was observed in about half of the objective Test of Everyday Attention for Children sub-scales, in parent and clinician, but not teacher reported behavior scores, and none but one of the objective neuropsychological tests. Finally, Qian et al. [61] tested Dawson & Guare’s 2010 [63] ecological executive skill straining program (Age = 8.3 years, n = 38 versus waitlist Age = 7.8 years, n = 30) to report, after 12 weekly 60 min sessions, significant improvements across self-reported symptoms and about half of self-reported executive function scales but not on objective assessments (e.g., Stroop Color-Word and Trail-Making Tests). As such, benefits from ecological training of executive function appear inconsistent, and in some cases driven by compensatory gains in participants with low baseline scores [64].

Metacognition in psychosocial interventions

Parental training

A relevant factor in efficacy of metacognitive training in children may include metacognitive support from parents. Three recent studies tested this hypothesis (Table 2), implementing 8–10 weeks of parental training with a 1–2-hour weekly session that explicitly included elements of metacognitive awareness, and reported significant benefits in measures of child-parent relationship, parental satisfaction, as well as in daily routine efficiency and ADHD symptoms [65–67]. Namely, Frisch et al. [65] enrolled 39 families (vs 33 waitlist families in cross-over design) to participate in a parental occupational executive training program (POET, 8 weekly sessions), which combined training in what executive functions are and how symptoms arise (e.g., impulsivity and delayed planning), and occupational solutions. Parents were explicitly instructed to raise awareness in children through simple explanations of the learned concepts. Benefits were significant in parent-reported ability to complete daily routines (e.g., morning-evening) and impulsivity (but not inattentive) symptoms, maintained in an 8–12-week follow-up. Second, Shah et al. [67] designed an adaptation of existing parent training approaches founded in operant conditioning and social learning theory (e.g., contingency management and behavior management techniques) [68–70], to incorporate values culturally appropriate in India. Doing so involved adding a biological perspective, home role-playing tasks to improve generalization, incorporated extended family in line with collectivistic values, and spiritual methods like yoga and meditation to reduce stress, that together may be viewed as adding metacognitive awareness into the training. The 10-week training program (n = 41) produced significant benefits in symptoms (inattentive & hyperactive) and school performance & classroom behavior, with benefits comparable regardless of whether the child was on medication, thus highlighting potential complementary value of benefits. It is notable that the benefits were reliable despite average completion rate being 6.89/10 sessions. Finally, Hahn-Markowitz et al. [66] reported significant increases in parent self-efficacy across 5/7 measured constructs, following a 10-week cognitive functional training program that uses metacognitive learning of executive strategies (e.g., stop, do/persist, check, plan) (n = 50/group, cross-over design). This result is consistent with parent-reported increases in awareness, reduction in guilt/blame and stress, and improvement in parent-child relationships & self-reliance, reported by Shah et al. [67] and others [71,72].

TABLE 2
Table 2. Metacognitive elements in psychosocial interventions in ADHD.

It is notable that the trainings did not improve inattentive symptoms in one study [65], and anxiety/depression scores in another [67] (also see CBT training Table 2), perhaps suggesting that parent training is less effective at scaffolding cognition or internalizing behaviors, than social or externalizing behaviors. If so, it may serve as a complement to programs that target metacognitive and/or skill training in children, as described in the previous section. Though research to address this question is lacking, relevant results were published by Tamm et al. [72] who, in a feasibility study, incorporated parent training within a combined metacognitive and executive skill training program in a small sample of children with combined-type ADHD (Age = 5.2 years, n = 24). The program included 60 min group training sessions, for 8 weeks, in which naturalistic activities were used to teach executive function skills, along with metacognitive reflection on skill understanding and performance strategies. Parents met in groups for analogous training that included metacognitive reflection on situations in which skills may be needed, and practiced implementation to be repeated in the home. Significant improvements were reported in 3/7 objective assessments of visual attention (NEPSY [77]), and in self-reported symptoms and scales of executive function. However, as a control group was not included, replications are warranted. Notably, and consistent with reports by others [67], the parent satisfaction ratings were high, with average rating of >5 on a 7-point Likert scale and 96% completion rate. In sum, this work supports the notion that parent-child metacognitive training has the potential to produce a mutualistic relationship that can support skill training in ADHD, while highlighting the need for further research with emphasis on objective outcomes and appropriate controls.

CBT in Adult ADHD

In the case of adult ADHD, reports of treatments targeting metacognitive awareness are fewer and nested within the context of cognitive behavioral training programs (CBT). It is notable that some have debated whether such approaches truly address metacognition [78]. However, as articulated by Moritz et al. [79], it is difficult to dispute that CBT approaches, by nature of including self-reflection and experience of targeted cognitions or behaviors, satisfy prevalent definitions of metacognition [31] by way of fostering metacognitive knowledge and experience. Thus, the debate may be in terms of degree to which a given CBT program incorporates metacognitive awareness, though operationalizing which programs do or do not include metacognitive elements is a clear challenge. Here we review programs that explicitly discuss metacognitive awareness or strategy or assess metacognition in outcomes.

In ADHD, CBT programs that meet these criteria include Solanto et al. [73–75] and Safren et al. [76] (Table 2). Solanto et al. [73] developed a 12-week (2hr/week) CBT that focused on time management, organization, and planning skills, and integrated discussion of and reflection on cognitive-behavioral strategies to promote metacognitive awareness. Compared to supportive therapy (n = 43) that lacked such discussions, the treatment group (n = 45) improved on Adult ADHD Investigator Symptoms Rating Scale [80] assessment of inattention, Conners inattention and memory scales, as well as time management sub-scales and self-rating executive functions scores. Improvement was significantly larger than in the supportive therapy group (p < 0.05). The treatment group also showed lower attrition (16% vs 37%) and higher responder rates (42.2% vs 12%) than the supportive therapy group [73]. A randomized controlled trial (RCT) of the program was subsequently performed in older adults with analogous results [74]. More recently the program was adapted to academic environment needs of college students with positive results in a feasibility trial that did not include a control group [75], including replication of low attrition (17%) and 67% completion rate for home work exercises, previously shown to improve treatment outcome [73]. A comparable CBT program was described by Safren et al. [76,81] which focused on the training of “compensatory strategies in organizing and planning, coping with distractibility and enhancing optimal thinking strategies”, reporting decreases in ADHD symptom severity (effect size = 1.2) and clinical global impression (effect size = 1.4), and 56% responder rate in the treatment group (n = 16, medication + CBT) when compared to a group on only pharmacological treatment (n = 15, responder rate 13%). In sum, the existing data suggest positive effects from CBT that incorporates metacognitive awareness strategies in adults with ADHD, though the small number of adequately powered and controlled RCTs, and lack of objective measures in this domain warrant further studies.

Metacognition in mindfulness training

An additional domain of treatment in ADHD that features elements of metacognition is that of mindfulness and meditation training. Mindfulness meditation techniques aim to foster awareness to one’s present experience, through silent sitting, walking or purposeful attention to daily activities. Such programs thus engage intentional self-regulation of attention [82,83], which falls within the definition of metacognitive awareness. Unlike the context of skill training or psychosocial interventions, mindfulness training does not target a specific ADHD symptom or skill. However, ADHD treatments based on mindfulness meditation activities have been developed, in part motivated by proposed associations between mindfulness and neural systems of attention [82,84], and could offer a valuable assessment of whether generalized awareness training is sufficient to improve ADHD skills over skill training. Numerous systematic reviews and meta-analysis have been published on the efficacy of mindfulness-based training programs in ADHD [30,85–91], and thus we focus here on summarizing key trends. Across meta-analyses, mindfulness and meditation training programs report significant reductions in ADHD symptoms (Hedges’ g = −0.17 to 4.03 [85] with most studies in the medium to large effect size range), with comparable, large effect sizes across self-reported and other-reported symptoms [90]. In three meta-analyses, the effect size was reported as larger in adults than children [30,88,90]. However, assessment of evidence quality suggest high bias is prevalent across reviewed studies [30,87,89,91], leading some to conclude that despite significant effects the evidence is methodologically insufficient to support meditation-based therapies for ADHD [30]. As such, data on the efficacy of mindfulness in ADHD is inconclusive, warranting further study.

Role of metacognitive ability

Finally, it is of note that metacognitive training, in any context, relies on metacognitive abilities. Thus, it is pertinent to ask if metacognitive training in ADHD bestows benefits via the training of metacognitive ability itself or if it scaffolds skill training, in line with the supportive role of metacognition in self-regulated learning (e.g., by way of engaging executive functions like planning [92] or by increasing resilience to anxiety through motivation [93]). One way to address this question is to ask, is metacognitive ability preserved in ADHD or does it differ, along with executive function performance with which it is intricately intertwined [94,95]. At first glance the findings are mixed. One recent study of 7–14-year-old children (n = 60 with ADHD), reported that scores on the Metacognitive Awareness Inventory were significantly lower in kids with ADHD than those without and correlated negatively with symptoms of ADHD as well as depression and anxiety. Yet, two other studies, report no group differences in metacognition. Slobodin et al. [96], also in 7–10 year old children with ADHD (n = 190), found that self-ratings of inattention correlated with objective performance scores on a sustained attention task, suggesting accurate self-awareness. Similarly, in adults, Butzbach et al. [97] (n = 47 ADHD, n = 47 non-ADHD) replicated performance deficits in attention in ADHD but did not find group differences in ability to estimate own performance in executive function, and memory. A small effect-size for over-estimation of attention was reported in participants with ADHD.

Thus, a global deficit in metacognition in ADHD is not supported by existing data. Further clarity on the discrepant findings may be gained from studies that use componential or multi-dimensional assessments of metacognition. For instance, Pezzica et al. [98] used a Children’s Awareness of Attention through Drawing tool to assess metacognition in 92 primary school age kids (n = 45 with ADHD, 5–11 years). They coded visual representations of attentive and inattentive constructs in children across five dimensions of awareness (behavioral, pragmatic, cognitive, social, and emotional). They found that children with and without ADHD had similar representations of attention versus inattention suggesting similar metacognitive constructs. They differed however in ability to organize educational tools and in the emotional state associated with school, with more negative emotion in visual representations of school in children with ADHD. This difference emerged with age (8–11 > 5–8 years). This finding supported the conclusion that in children with ADHD, negative feelings around attention increase with age, potentially through social learning and self-awareness around the deficits. This conclusion is consistent with the results of an adult study (n = 40 ADHD, n = 42 non-ADHD), in which a sub-component analysis of responses on the Metacognitions Questionnaire-30 (MCQ-30) revealed that only the score on the confidence subscale of the MCQ-30 predicted inattentive scale scores, and there were no group differences in a score of general cognitive consciousness. These studies suggest that metacognition in individuals with and without ADHD is not reliably different, and thus metacognitive training may exert positive effects through scaffolding of executive function or motivational processes needed for behavioral management.

OUTSTANDING QUESTIONS & CONCLUSIONS

The reviewed data highlight both that metacognitive approaches have potential in supporting symptom management in ADHD, and that much research in this domain is needed and warranted. The premise that metacognition may serve to support symptom management is bolstered by evidence of metacognitive awareness in individuals with ADHD. Educational context, where research reports are most numerous and report positive effects in objective assessments and near and far-transfer following training, offers a valuable foundation for future research designs that include objective quantifiable outcomes. Notable methodological gaps include refinements on the relative contributions of metacognitive elements versus skill training in combined approaches, continued development of objective assessments, especially in psychosocial approaches such as parental training and CBT (cf., Table 2), and systematic assessment of metacognitive training in adult ADHD, which is sparse. The need for objective outcomes is particularly important for reducing attrition rates in self-report of symptoms, which reduces the impact of long-term follow up studies [65,67].

Furthermore, given increasing notice of gender differences in ADHD symptoms and outcomes [99–102], the role of gender in effects of metacognitive training in this population is an important understudied research goal. In the reviewed studies, a bias towards males was present across study samples (Table 1 mean = 33% females, Table 2 mean = 40% females). Though 5 of the 17 reviewed studies included gender as a covariate or moderating variable and found no significant effects [60,61,65,66,73], the role of gender will require additional systematic study especially in larger samples. Similarly, the effect of age on efficacy of metacognitive training warrants further study. In the reviewed studies significant results were reported in children as young as five, though with the scaffolding provided by the educator or parent potentially playing a significant part. Finally, it is notable that metacognition may play an important role in shaping other existing alternative treatments such as neurofeedback or some digital interventions (e.g., brain training), that do not currently involve metacognition and thus are not considered in the reviewed works. In conclusion, metacognitive training, with focus on awareness and self-regulation, both shows clear potential to support symptom management and warrants research across several domains, thus defining a distinct frontier in the field of ADHD treatments.

DATA AVAILABILITY

No data were generated from the study.

AUTHORS’ CONTRIBUTIONS

AL, BD, and GS conceived the study. AL wrote the manuscript. AL, BD, GS edited the manuscript.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

AL has received payments as a consultant and holds shares in Think Now, Inc. BDS and GVS are officers of Think Now, Inc. and hold shares in Think Now, Inc. The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

FUNDING

This works was supported by funding from the National Institutes of Health to AL (MH116268, MH128475, MH130731) and to GVS (MH122109).

REFERENCES

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

23.

24.

25.

26.

27.

28.

29.

30.

31.

32.

33.

34.

35.

36.

37.

38.

39.

40.

41.

42.

43.

44.

45.

46.

47.

48.

49.

50.

51.

52.

53.

54.

55.

56.

57.

58.

59.

60.

61.

62.

63.

64.

65.

66.

67.

68.

69.

70.

71.

72.

73.

74.

75.

76.

77.

78.

79.

80.

81.

82.

83.

84.

85.

86.

87.

88.

89.

90.

91.

92.

93.

94.

95.

96.

97.

98.

99.

100.

101.

102.

How to Cite This Article

Lenartowicz A, DeSchepper B, Simpson GB. Training of Awareness in ADHD: Leveraging Metacognition. J Psychiatry Brain Sci. 2024;9:e240006. https://doi.org/10.20900/jpbs.20240006

Copyright © 2023 Hapres Co., Ltd. Privacy Policy | Terms and Conditions